Variación intencional en tareas de Teoría de la Mente Moralmente relevante (MoToM)

Autores/as

  • Jesús Goenaga Universidad de San Buenaventura, Seccional Medellín, Colombia. https://orcid.org/0000-0003-1936-1642
  • Daniel Sierra Universidad de San Buenaventura seccional Medellín
  • Federico Gallego Universidad de San Buenaventura, Seccional Medellín, Colombia.
  • Karen Artunduaga Universidad de San Buenaventura, Seccional Medellín, Colombia.

DOI:

https://doi.org/10.18270/chps..v18i2.3050

Palabras clave:

juicio moral, Teoría de la Mente (ToM), Teoría de la Mente Moralmente Relevante (MoToM), lectura intencional, tarea de falsa creencia

Resumen

La relación entre Teoría de la Mente (ToM) y juicio moral permitió el diseño de tareas de Teoría de la Mente Moralmente Relevantes (MoToM), las cuales integran la lectura intencional a la elaboración de dichos juicios. Este trabajo describe la producción de una variación de la MoToM en el juicio moral con lectura intencional en niños de 5 y 8 años, quienes aplicaron una tarea de MoToM y variación de esta, otorgando dos estimaciones del juicio moral, antes y después de la variación. Se encontraron diferencias significativas entre edades ante la elaboración del juicio. Se confirmó la relación entre ToM y juicio moral, reconociendo otros aspectos del desarrollo, los cuales son fundamentales para la integración de la intencionalidad en la elaboración de juicios.

Descargas

Los datos de descargas todavía no están disponibles.

Referencias bibliográficas

Abu-Akel, A. (2018). The case for simulation theory and theory- theory as interaction accounts of theory of Mind. Constructivist Foundations, 14(1), 33-34. https://constructivist.info/14/1/033.abu-akel

Apperly, I. A. (2008). Beyond simulation–theory and theory–theory: why social cognitive neuroscience should use its own concepts to study “Theory of Mind”. Cognition, 107(1), 266-283. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2007.07.019

Ball, C. L., Smetana, J. G., & Sturge‐Apple, M. L. (2017). Following my head and my heart: Integrating preschoolers' empathy, theory of mind, and moral judgments. Child Development, 88(2), 597-611. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12605

Barclay, P. (2015). Reputation. En Buss, D (Ed) The Handbook of Evolutionary Psychology, (pp. 810-828). Wiley. http://www.patbarclay.com/wp-content/uploads/2015/08/Barclay-Reputation-sending-version.pdf

Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind”? Cognition, 21(1), 37-46. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(85)90022-8

Bloom, P., & German, T. P. (2000). Two reasons to abandon the false belief task as a test of theory of mind. Cognition, 77(1), 25-31. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0010-0277(00)00096-2

Butterfill, S. A., & Apperly, I. A. (2013). How to construct a minimal theory of mind. Mind & Language, 28(5), 606-637 https://doi.org/10.1111/mila.12036

Bzdok, D., Schilbach, L., Vogeley, K., Schneider, K., Laird, A. R., Langner, R., & Eickhoff, S. B. (2012). Parsing the neural correlates of moral cognition: ALE meta-analysis on morality, theory of mind, and empathy. Brain Structure and Function, 217(4), 783-796. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00429-012-0380-y

Chakroff, A., Dungan, J., Koster-Hale, J., Brown, A., Saxe, R., & Young, L. (2016). When minds matter for moral judgment: intent information is neurally encoded for harmful but not impure acts. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 11(3), 476-484. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsv131

Cushman, F., Sheketoff, R., Wharton, S., & Carey, S. (20 13). The development of intent-based moral judgment. Cognition, 127(1), 6-21. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2012.11.008

D'Esterre, A. P., Rizzo, M. T., & Killen, M. (2019). Unintentional and intentional falsehoods: The role of morally relevant theory of mind. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 177, 53-69. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2018.07.013

Fu, G., Xiao, W. S., Killen, M., & Lee, K. (2014). Moral judgment and its relation to second-order theory of mind. Developmental Psychology, 50(8), 2085. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0037077

Geipel, J., Hadjichristidis, C., & Surian, L. (2016). Foreign language affects the contribution of intentions and outcomes to moral judgment. Cognition, 154, 34-39. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2016.05.010

Goenaga, J. (2016). Revisión sistemática sobre juicio moral en toma de decisiones morales (2005-2015). Tesis de Maestría en Psicología. Medellín: Departamento de Psicología, Universidad de Antioquia.

Greene, J., & Haidt, J. (2002). How (and where) does moral judgment work? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6(12), 517-523. https://doi.org/10.1016/s1364-6613(02)02011-9

Greene, J. D., Sommerville, R. B., Nystrom, L. E., Darley, J. M., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). An fMRI investigation of emotional engagement in moral judgment. Science, 293(5537), 2105-2108. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1062872

Haidt, J. (2001). The emotional dog and its rational tail: A social intuitionist approach to moral judgment. Psychological Review, 108, 814-834. https://doi.org/10.1017/cbo9780511814273.055

Killen, M., Mulvey, K. L., Richardson, C., Jampol, N., & Woodward, A. (2011). The
accidental transgressor: Morally-relevant theory of mind. Cognition, 119(2), 197-215. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2011.01.006

Koster-Hale, J., Saxe, R., Dungan, J., & Young, L. L. (2013). Decoding moral judgments from neural representations of intentions. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 201207992. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1207992110

Kuntoro, I. A., Dwiputri, G., & Adams, P. (2018). The contribution of parenting style and theory of mind to the understanding of morally relevant theory of mind in Indonesian children. Diversity in Unity: Perspectives from Psychology and Behavioral Sciences, 83 - 89. https://doi.org/10.1201/9781315225302-11

Lane, J. D., Wellman, H. M., Olson, S. L., LaBounty, J., & Kerr, D. C. (2010). Theory of mind and emotion understanding predict moral development in early childhood. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 28(4), 871-889. https://doi.org/10.1348/026151009x483056

Li, L., Rizzo, M. T., Burkholder, A. R., & Killen, M. (2017). Theory of mind and resource allocation in the context of hidden inequality. Cognitive Development, 43, 25-36. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2017.02.001

Margoni, F., & Surian, L. (2017). Children’s intention-based moral judgments of
helping agents. Cognitive Development, 41, 46-64. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2016.12.001

Miller, S. A. (2009). Children’s understanding of second-order mental states. Psychological Bulletin, 135(5), 749. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0016854

Monin, B., Pizarro, D. A., & Beer, J. S. (2007). Deciding versus reacting: Conceptions of moral judgment and the reason-affect debate. Review of general psychology, 11(2), 99-111. https://doi.org/10.1037/1089-2680.11.2.99

Moran, J. M. (2013). Lifespan development: The effects of typical aging on theory of mind. Behavioural Brain Research, 237, 32-40. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2012.09.020

Nobes, G., Panagiotaki, G., & Pawson, C. (2009). The influence of negligence, intention, and outcome on children’s moral judgments. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 104(4), 382-397. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2009.08.001

Olson, K. R., & Spelke, E. S. (2008). Foundations of cooperation in young children. Cognition, 108(1), 222-231. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2007.12.003

Ortiz, D. S. U., Botero, M. G., & Tobón, O. E. A. (2010). Teoría de la mente: una revisión acerca del desarrollo del concepto. Revista Colombiana de Ciencias Sociales, 1(1), 28-37. https://www.funlam.edu.co/revistas/index.php/RCCS/article/view/1169

Reddy, V., & Morris, P. (2004). Participants don’t need theories: knowing minds in engagement. Theory & Psychology, 14(5), 647-665. https://doi.org/10.1057/9780230234383_5

Smetana, J. G., Jambon, M., Conry-Murray, C., & Sturge-Apple, M. L. (2012). Reciprocal Associations Between Young Children's Developing Moral Judgments and Theory of Mind. Developmental Psychology, 48(4), 1144-1155. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0025891

Sodian, B., Licata, M., Kristen‐Antonow, S., Paulus, M., Killen, M., & Woodward, A. (2016). Understanding of goals, beliefs, and desires predicts morally relevant theory of mind: A longitudinal investigation. Child Development, 87(4), 1221-1232. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12533

Tirapu-Ustárroz, J., Pérez-Sayes, G., Erekatxo-Bilbao, M., & Pelegrín-Valero, C. (2007). ¿Qué es la teoría de la mente? Revista de Neurología, 44(8), 479-489. https://doi.org/10.33588/rn.4408.2006295

Tomasello, M., & Vaish, A. (2013). Origins of human cooperation and morality. Annual review of psychology, 64, 231-255. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143812

Vaish, A., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2010). Young children selectively avoid helping people with harmful intentions. Child Development, 81(6),
1661-1669. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01500.x

Wimmer, H., & Perner, J. (1983). Beliefs about beliefs: Representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children's understanding of deception. Cognition, 13(1), 103-128. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(83)90004-5

Young, L., Cushman, F., Hauser, M., & Saxe, R. (2007). The neural basis of the interaction between theory of mind and moral judgment. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104(20), 8235-8240. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0701408104

Young, L., & Saxe, R. (2008). The neural basis of belief encoding and integration in moral judgment. Neuroimage, 40(4), 1912-1920. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.01.057

Young, L., Camprodon, J. A., Hauser, M., Pascual-Leone, A., & Saxe, R. (2010). Disruption of the right temporoparietal junction with transcranial magnetic stimulation reduces the role of beliefs in moral judgments. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107(15), 6753-6758. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0914826107

Descargas

Publicado

2020-06-05

Cómo citar

Goenaga, J., Sierra, D., Gallego, F., & Artunduaga, K. (2020). Variación intencional en tareas de Teoría de la Mente Moralmente relevante (MoToM). Cuadernos Hispanoamericanos De Psicología, 18(2), 1–16. https://doi.org/10.18270/chps.v18i2.3050